When experts get it spectacularly wrong

Relying on expert judgement is a risky but necessary business, requiring the right tools and the right experts

Dr Daryl Holland, University of Melbourne

Published 2 July 2015

Society relies on the judgement of experts, and when experts are wrong – bad things can happen.

“Experts are error-prone, over-confident and prey to a host of motivational and contextual biases,” says University of Melbourne quantitative ecologist Professor Mark Burgman.

Cane toads were released to help the cane industry deal with insect attacks on sugar cane roots. Picture: National Museum Australia

He has just published a book, Trusting Judgements: How to Get the Best out of Experts, and coauthored an article for Nature with Professor William Sutherland from the University of Cambridge, calling on policymakers to act on the scientific evidence that their expert advisers are often wrong.

They can have high status, they can be very experienced, they can appear credible and be spectacularly wrong.

Professor Burgman, the Director of the Centre of Excellence for Biosecurity Risk Analysis (CEBRA) in the School of Biosciences, says history is littered with examples where expert judgement was wrong, from the prediction that cane toads would not damage the Australian environment to the conclusion that Iraq had weapons of mass destruction in 2003.

“It turns out that in some domains, like trying to predict geopolitical outcomes, or trying to predict the outcomes of investments in the stock market, you could do just as well by tossing a coin,” he says.

He says that in most cases, expert judgements do have value, but only when put in the right context.

First, he says, you need to ask the right questions, and second, you can’t ask just one expert.

“If you ask a group of experts in a structured, deliberate fashion, then you’ll get an answer that is closer to the truth and better calibrated than if you ask the best regarded person in the room. And that improvement will be considerable and consistent,” Professor Burgman says.

In fact, research increasingly shows the eminence of an expert is a poor indicator of their performance in predicting future outcomes.

President George W. Bush relied on expert judgement about Weapons of Mass Destruction for the 2003 Iraq war. Picture: Getty Images

“We were involved in an experiment with the US intelligence service over the past five years and there were five groups working on this project independently,” Professor Burgman says.

“The one thing that was common to all of the groups is that the standard indicators of expert performance – which are experience, publications, memberships, status and other things of that nature – are no guide to a person’s ability to predict or judge an unknown fact. The only thing that provides us with a guide is the person’s prior performance on similar questions.

“So, if the person’s been really good in the past it’s likely they’ll be pretty good in the future. And we use that as a guide to weigh their judgements.”

The main focus of research at CEBRA is biosecurity risk.

Professor Burgman says it is imperative that predictions in this area are as accurate as possible.

“One case of foot-and-mouth disease could cost the Australian economy four billion dollars, even if we eliminated that one case instantly,” he says.

“We rather think that if you’re not using these tools in areas that involve public policy, health and safety, security or biosecurity, than really you’re being professionally negligent.”

Professor Burgman and his team are now trying to adapt these techniques to ecology and conservation biology, a field where expert judgement has had a checkered past.

“The introduction of cane toads was made on the basis of expert judgement in the 1930s,” Professor Burgman says.

One case of foot-and-mouth disease could cost the Australian economy four billion dollars. Picture: Getty Images

“A very confident scientist said there was no evidence there would be any negative impacts, on the basis of observations they’d made overseas. And of course we now know the introduction of cane toads has been an unmitigated disaster.

“The decision to bring Sumatran rhinos into captivity in the 1980s was made on the basis of expert judgements about the proficiency of the people who were going to capture these things and the effectiveness of zoos to generate captive breeding programs.

“It was wonderfully unsuccessful and it did more harm than good.”

Professor Burgman’s group recently helped the Australian Government’s Threatened Species Scientific Committee develop protocols to make better judgements about risk to threatened species.

The group will continue to develop these tools as part of the University of Melbourne’s contribution to the National Environmental Science Programme (NESP) Threatened Species Recovery Hub.

Professor Burgman says they have applied to the Australian Research Council for funding to extend their work to other areas of ecology and conservation biology.

“We’d like a consortium of philosophers and mathematicians and psychologists and ecologists. We could do with 50 people.”

Find out more about research in this faculty

Science

Content Card Slider


Content Card Slider


Subscribe for your weekly email digest

By subscribing, you agree to our

Acknowledgement of country

We acknowledge Aboriginal and Torres Strait Islander people as the Traditional Owners of the unceded lands on which we work, learn and live. We pay respect to Elders past, present and future, and acknowledge the importance of Indigenous knowledge in the Academy.

Read about our Indigenous priorities
Phone: 13 MELB (13 6352) | International: +61 3 9035 5511The University of Melbourne ABN: 84 002 705 224CRICOS Provider Code: 00116K (visa information)